Literacy as an Outcome of Language Development and its Impact on Children’s Psychosocial and Emotional Development


1University of Pittsburgh, USA, 2University of Iowa, USA
, Rev. ed.

PDF version

Introduction

One of the most striking accomplishments of the preschool years is the child’s development of speech and language. As children enter school, they are expected to use these newly developed language skills as tools for learning and social negotiation. The important role of spoken and written communication in school-aged children’s lives suggests that individual differences in these skills may entail benefits and risks, in terms of broader academic and psychosocial competence.

Subject

Spoken language competence involves several systems. Children must master a system for representing meaning, and acquire a facility with the forms of language, ranging from the sound structure of words to the grammatical structure of sentences. This knowledge must be joined with their social competence. Much of this learning is accomplished without formal instruction, and what is known is largely tacit in nature. Preschool children begin to develop some awareness of this knowledge by rhyming words, for example, or taking a word apart into syllables. This ability to think about the sounds in words is called phonological awareness. Early reading development in alphabetic languages such as English depends on the integrity of phonological awareness and other related phonological processing abilities. 

Learning to read also requires several skills. It is common to differentiate between two main aspects of reading: word recognition and comprehension. Word recognition consists of knowing how a word is pronounced.  Early in reading development, children need to recognize letters, be aware of and able to manipulate sounds within words, and use conventions about the relationship between letters and their pronunciation. In addition, the child needs to be able to interpret the meaning of the printed text. The skills involved in this aspect of reading are very similar to those used in listening comprehension.  

Although word recognition and comprehension are often considered separately, they can influence one another over development, in a bidirectional way. For example, vocabulary knowledge contributes directly to growth in word recognition,2,3 and later in the school years, skill in word recognition predicts the rate of vocabulary growth.4

Problems

Children may enter school with poor skills in listening, speaking and/or phonological processing. Children with poor listening and speaking skills are referred to as having a language impairment (LI) or developmental language disorder (DLD): current estimates are that about 10% of children entering schools in the U.S. and Canada have LI.5-7 There are other children for whom phonological processing skills are poor, and these children are at risk for reading disorder (RD). Estimates of the prevalence of RD among school-aged children typically range between 10 and 18%.8-10 While LI and RD in the early school years can occur alone, it is very common for a child to be affected by both. 

In addition to academic difficulties, several studies have shown elevated rates of behaviour problems among children with LI, including externalizing and internalizing problems, and have an especially strong relationship with ADHD.5,11-27 Similarly, children with a diagnosis of behavioural or emotional disorder very frequently meet the criteria for LI, as do incarcerated youth.26,28-30 Language impairments are commonly undiagnosed in these children, possibly because professionals are not sensitive to the manifestations of language impairments in this group.30 Another concerning social outcome for individuals with language impairment is an elevated risk of victimization, including sexual assault.31,32 

Research Context

The relationships between spoken language development, reading development and social development have been explored by several researchers in an effort to determine the extent to which these problems are associated with each other and the bases for these relationships.

Key Research Questions

The prominent research questions have been concerned with the extent to which aspects of early language status are predictive of later reading and behaviour problems and what the possible bases might be for these relationships. Specifically, two hypotheses have figured prominently in the literature. One hypothesis is that the associations between spoken language and later outcomes are causal. Alternatively, the association of language and reading problems with behaviour problems may rest on a common underlying condition such as a neuromaturational delay that results in poor achievement in both domains.  

Recent Research Results

It is clear that as a group, children with LI have poorer reading achievement and higher rates of RD.33-37 In these studies, the prevalence of RD in children with LI ranged from 25%8 to 90%.11 The strong relationship between RD and LI has been shown to be attributable to the limitations these children have in both their ability to understand language and their phonological awareness.13,14,38-41 The phonological-awareness deficits place them at risk for difficulties in learning decoding skills and the comprehension problems place them at risk for reading comprehension problems.

There are several possible causal relationships between language and behavioural disorders: language difficulties might lead to reactive behaviour problems, behaviour problems could lead to fewer opportunities for language learning, or the relationship between language and behavioural difficulties could be bidirectional.18 During the school years, children with poor language may encounter difficulties with the spoken and written communication demands of the classroom, such that communication failure, especially in the classroom, serves as a stressor, and behaviour problems are maladaptive responses to this stressor. In support of this notion, behaviour problems are reported by the children’s teachers to a greater degree than their parents. Furthermore, teacher ratings of behaviour problems correspond more closely than parent ratings to child’s language test scores.17,33,42 Further support for this view arises from data showing that among children with LI, reading disorder further increases the risk of behaviour disorder. This supports the notion that LI in conjunction with RD results in the child facing excessive failure, particularly within the classroom, which in turn results in reactive behaviour problems. These conclusions, however, fail to explain why behaviour problems seem to be reported in preschool children with LI,22 a finding that could be used to argue for an underlying factor such as neurodevelopmental delay that contributes to all these conditions. Another possibility is a bidirectional relationship between language and behavioural difficulties. This idea is supported by evidence that language difficulties at age three increase the risk of conduct disorders at age five, and vice versa.23 In particular, children with language impairment are vulnerable to difficulties with self-regulation, which may in turn lead to observed behavioural difficulties.27,43,44 

Research Gaps

Further research efforts are needed, focusing on the particular mechanisms that produce this complex of spoken, written and behaviour problems. Several recent studies have addressed the question of whether certain profiles of language weaknesses are associated with different types of behavioural outcomes.24,25,31,45-49 This approach seems promising, as it could help focus interventions on the communication skills that are most likely to affect important outcomes. There is also a need for classroom-based studies of how children with language difficulties respond to communication demands and failure. Finally, given the risk of adverse outcomes such as incarceration or victimization, there is a need to continue to identify experiences and skills that contribute to resilience in children with early language difficulties.48-50 

Conclusions

The existence of a strong relationship between spoken language skills and subsequent reading and behaviour development is generally supported in the literature. The basis of the relationship between early spoken language and later reading development is thought to be causal in nature, such that spoken language skills, especially phonological awareness and listening comprehension, are fundamental precursors to later successful reading. Children with limitations in phonological processing are at risk for early decoding problems, which can then lead to problems of reading comprehension. Children with problems of listening comprehension are at risk for reading comprehension problems even if they can decode words.  These skills can also dynamically interact over development.

The basis of the relationship between spoken language and later behaviour problems is less clear, although it seems possible that there are multiple mechanisms that could explain the relationship. In particular, academic difficulties that result from LI may contribute to the increased risk of behavioural disorders.  

Implications

The evidence is compelling that a foundation in spoken language competence is important for the successful achievement of academic and social competence. Children with poor language skills are therefore at risk for reading and psychosocial problems. Language difficulties could be identified efficiently at school entry. This identification process should be an especially high priority for children who already show signs of behavioural difficulties, given the high incidence and low identification of language difficulties in this group. Interventions are available for promoting language growth, and in particular, numerous programs exist to promote phonological awareness. Additionally, intervention efforts need to focus on approaches that provide supportive educational environments, to reduce the stressors that may result in maladaptive behaviours. Finally, early intervention efforts are warranted, to support the development of language skills prior to school entry.  Where successful, such efforts could be expected to reduce a child’s risk of important academic and psychosocial difficulties throughout childhood, and into adulthood.

References

  1. Snow CE, Burns MS, Griffin P, eds. Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 1998.
  2. Oullette G, Beers A. A not-so-simple view of reading: how oral vocabulary and visual-word recognition complicate the story. Reading and Writing 2010;23(2):189-208.
  3. Cain K. Learning to read: why should we keep things simple? Reading Research Quarterly 2015;50:151-169.
  4. Duff D, Tomblin JB, Catts H. The Influence of Reading on Vocabulary Growth: A Case for a Matthew Effect. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 2015;58(3):853-864.
  5. Beitchman JH, Nair R, Clegg M, Patel PG. Prevalence of speech and language disorders in 5-year-old kindergarten children in the Ottawa-Carleton region. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 1986;51:98-110.
  6. Tomblin JB, Records N, Buckwalter P, Zhang X, Smith E, O'Brien M. Prevalence of Specific Language Impairment in Kindergarten Children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 1997;40(6):1245-1260.
  7. Norbury CF, Gooch D, Wray C, et al. The impact of nonverbal ability on prevalence and clinical presentation of language disorder: evidence from a population study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2016;57(11):1247-1257.
  8. Hulme C, Snowling MJ. Children's Reading Comprehension Difficulties. Current Directions in Psychological Science 2011;20(3):139-142.
  9. Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA. Unlocking learning disabilities: The neurological basis. In: Cramer SCE, W., ed. Learning disabilities: Lifelong issues. Baltimore, MD: P. H. Brooks Publishing; 1996:255-260.
  10. Commission on Emotional and Learning Disorders in Children. One million children: A national study of Canadian children with emotional and learning disorders. Toronto, Ontario: Leonard Crainford; 1970.
  11. Beitchman JH, Hood J, Inglis A. Psychiatric risk in children with speech and language disorders. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 1990;18(3):283-296.
  12. Beitchman JH, Hood J, Rochon J, Peterson M. Empirical classification of speech/language impairment in children: II. Behavioral characteristics. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 1989;28(1):118-123.
  13. Beitchman J, Tuckett M, Batth S. Language delay and hyperactivity in preschoolers: evidence for a distinct subgroup of hyperactives. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 1987;32(8):683-687.
  14. Beitchman JH, Brownlie EB, Wilson B. Linguistic impairment and psychiatric disorder: Pathways to outcome. In: Beitchman JH, Cohen NJ, Konstantareas MM, Tannock R, eds. Language, learning, and behavior disorders: Developmental, biological, and clinical perspectives. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 1996:493-514.
  15. Stevenson J, Richman N, Graham PJ. Behaviour problems and language abilities at three years and behavioural deviance at eight years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 1985;26(2):215-230.
  16. Benasich AA, Curtiss S, Tallal P. Language, learning, and behavioral disturbances in childhood: A longitudinal perspective. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 1993;32(3):585-594.
  17. Tomblin JB. Educational and psychosocial outcomes of language impairment in kindergarten. In: Tomblin JB, Nippold MA, eds. Understanding individual differences in language development across the school years. New York, NY: Psychology Press; 2014:166-203.
  18. Mueller KL, Tomblin JB. Examining the comorbidity of language disorders and ADHD. Topics in Language Disorders 2012;32(3):228-246.
  19. Wadman R, Botting N, Durkin K, Conti-Ramsden G. Changes in emotional health symptoms in adolescents with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 2011;46(6):641-656.
  20. Mok PL, Pickles A, Durkin K, Conti-Ramsden G. Longitudinal trajectories of peer relations in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2014;55(5):516-527.
  21. Yew SG, O'Kearney R. Emotional and behavioural outcomes later in childhood and adolescence for children with specific language impairments: meta-analyses of controlled prospective studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2013;54(5):516-524.
  22. Conti-Ramsden G, Durkin K, Toseeb U, Botting N, Pickles A. Education and employment outcomes of young adults with a history of developmental language disorder. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 2018;53(2):237-255.
  23. Girard LC, Pingault JB, Doyle O, Falissard B, Tremblay RE. Developmental Associations Between Conduct Problems and Expressive Language in Early Childhood: A Population-Based Study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 2016;44(6):1033-1043.
  24. St Clair MC, Pickles A, Durkin K, Conti-Ramsden G. A longitudinal study of behavioral, emotional and social difficulties in individuals with a history of specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Communication Disorders 2011;44(2):186-199.
  25. Pickles A, Durkin K, Mok PLH, Toseeb U, Conti-Ramsden G. Conduct problems co-occur with hyperactivity in children with language impairment: A longitudinal study from childhood to adolescence. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments 2016;1. doi:10.1177/2396941516645251
  26. Islam UA, Poole KL, Schmidt LA, Ford J, Saigal S, Van Lieshout RJ. Childhood language skills and adolescent self-esteem in preterm survivors. Journal of Child Health Care 2018;22(1):34-45.
  27. Bornstein MH, Hahn CS, Suwalsky JT. Language and internalizing and externalizing behavioral adjustment: developmental pathways from childhood to adolescence. Development and Psychopathology 2013;25(3):857-878.
  28. Hughes N, Chitsabesan P, Bryan K, Borshmann R, Swain N, Lennox P, Shaw J. Language impairment and comorbid vulnerabilities among young people in custody. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2017;58(10):1106-1113.
  29. Moncrieff D, Miller E, Hill E. Screening tests reveal high risk among adjudicated adolescents of auditory processing and language disorders. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 2018;61(4):924-935.
  30. Chow JC, Hollo A. Language ability of students with emotional disturbance: Discrepancies between teacher ratings and direct assessment. Assessment for Effective Intervention 2018;43(2):90-95.
  31. Conti-Ramsden G, Botting N. Social difficulties and victimization in children with SLI at 11 years of age. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 2004;47:145-161.
  32. Brownlie EB, Graham E, Bao L, Koyama E, Beitchman JH. Language disorder and retrospectively reported sexual abuse of girls: severity and disclosure. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2017;58(10):1114-1121.
  33. Tomblin JB, Zhang X, Buckwalter P, Catts H. Association of reading disability behavioral disorders and language impairment among second grade children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2000;41(4):473-482.
  34. Aram DM, Ekelman BL, E. NJ. Preschoolers with language disorders: 10 years later. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 1984;27(2):232-244.
  35. Bishop DVM, Adams C. A prospective study of the relationship between specific language impairment, phonological disorders and reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 1990;31(7):1027-1050.
  36. Catts HW, Fey ME, Tomblin JB, Zhang X. A Longitudinal Investigation of Reading Outcomes in Children With Language Impairments. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 2002;45(6).
  37. Janus M, Labonté C, Kirkpatrick R, Davies S, Duku E. The impact of speech and language problems in kindergarten on academic learning and special education status in grade three. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 2017:1-14.
  38. Oakhill JV, Cain K. The Precursors of Reading Ability in Young Readers: Evidence From a Four-Year Longitudinal Study. Scientific Studies of Reading 2012;16(2):91-121.
  39. Suggate S, Schaughency E, McAnally H, Reese E. From infancy to adolescence: The longitudinal links between vocabulary, early literacy skills, oral narrative, and reading comprehension. Cognitive Development 2018;47:82-95.
  40. Elwer S, Keenan JM, Olson RK, Byrne B, Samuelsson S. Longitudinal stability and predictors of poor oral comprehenders and poor decoders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 2013;115(3):497-516.
  41. Snellings P, van der Leij A, de Jong PF, Blok H. Enhancing the reading fluency and comprehension of children with reading disabilities in an orthographically transparent language. Journal of Learning Disabilities 2009;42(4).
  42. Lindsay G, Dockrell J, Desforges M, Law J, Peacey N. Meeting the needs of children and young people with speech, language and communication difficulties. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 2010;45(4):448-460.
  43. Aro T, Laakso ML, Maatta S, Tolvanen A, Poikkeus AM. Associations between toddler-age communication and kindergarten-age self-regulatory skills. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 2014;57(4):1405-1417.
  44. Petersen IT, Bates JE, Staples AD. The role of language ability and self-regulation in the development of inattentive-hyperactive behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology 2015;27(1):221-237.
  45. Durkin K, Toseeb U, Botting N, Pickles A, Conti-Ramsden G. Social Confidence in Early Adulthood Among Young People With and Without a History of Language Impairment. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 2017;60(6):1635-1647.
  46. Zadeh ZY, Im-Bolter N, Cohen NJ. Social cognition and externalizing psychopathology: an investigation of the mediating role of language. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 2007;35(2):141-152.
  47. Lonigan CJ, Spiegel JA, Goodrich JM, et al. Does Preschool Self-Regulation Predict Later Behavior Problems in General or Specific Problem Behaviors? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 2017;45(8):1491-1502.
  48. Yew SG, O'Kearney R. Early language impairments and developmental pathways of emotional problems across childhood. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 2015;50(3):358-373.
  49. Yew SG, O'Kearney R. The Role of Early Language Difficulties in the Trajectories of Conduct Problems Across Childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 2015;43(8):1515-1527.
  50. Rhoad-Drogalis A, Sawyer BE, Justice LM, O’Connell AA. Assessing Learning Behaviors in Early Childhood Special Education Classrooms. Early Education and Development 2018;29(4):450-466.

How to cite this article:

Duff D, Tomblin BJ. Literacy as an Outcome of Language Development and its Impact on Children’s Psychosocial and Emotional Development. In: Tremblay RE, Boivin M, Peters RDeV, eds. Rvachew S, topic ed. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online]. https://www.child-encyclopedia.com/language-development-and-literacy/according-experts/literacy-outcome-language-development-and-its. Updated: October 2018. Accessed November 22, 2024.

Text copied to the clipboard ✓